Contiguity and
contingency may both be effective tools for the formation of associations.
Contiguity is literally, “the state of bordering or being in direct contact
with something.” In the world of psychology, contiguity is used to explain the
order or the closeness of stimulus and response in occurrence, leading to the
formation of association – or, simply, timing. Contingency, on the other hand,
is “a future event or circumstance that is possible but cannot be predicted
with certainty.” In psychology, contingency is simplified as the reliability of
the association. Both are useful tools in the formation of associations, but
the question is whether or not one of these tools is more useful than the
other, or if they are both necessary to utilize when forming associations in
the mind.
Contiguity of associations is very straightforward. If a
behavior is rewarded, it is more likely to be repeated. If the behavior is
rewarded quickly after the behavior occurs, it is even more likely to be
repeated. It has been observed that continuously delaying delivery of
reinforcement tends to result in a decrease in the target behavior. Delayed
learning can occur, however, if little activity interferes between the target
response and the reinforcer. Edward Thorndike believed that temporal contiguity
is a critical feature of the response-reinforcer relationship. Pavlov agreed,
to a certain extent, but believed that the CS-US contiguity alone may not be
enough. Superstition is a part of contiguity in learning. Skinner first
observed this behavior in pigeons that were displaying interim behavior in-between
regular temporal intervals in which Skinner gave the pigeons food. Each of the
pigeons developed a different behavior in an attempt to get the reward, because
each pigeon was most likely doing something different when they were rewarded
previously.
Contingency is most easily seen in correlations between
the conditioned stimulus and the unconditioned stimulus. Contingency is
important in anticipation of an event, as it helps to form the “if-then”
relationship in the association. In contingency learning, the signal serves
only to change the predictability of the context. By making it less
predictable, the signal reduces the extent to which the context competes with
the operant response in behavior. Contingency is observed in learned
helplessness. When the animal gives up any hope of being able to save itself
from the persistent shocks, it simply gives up. The contingency is observed in
the animal believing that the shocks are coming, no matter what.
Contiguity and contingency are both suitable tools for
learning. Contingency, as seen in the example of learned helplessness, is
sufficient for learning. Contiguity, however, appears to be necessary for
learning. It is possible for delayed learning to occur, but it is not nearly as
productive for learning as a speedy association formed with a timely reward.
Temporal contiguity is the most efficient way to reinforce behavior.
Nice. But it seems that contiguity is not ever *necessary*!
ReplyDeleteWhen a CS predicts the *omission* of an otherwise to be expected US, there is not two contiguous stimuli presented to the animal!
*even* not ever!
ReplyDelete